Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item:
https://hdl.handle.net/2445/114816
Full metadata record
DC Field | Value | Language |
---|---|---|
dc.contributor.author | Fornells Ambrojo, Miriam | - |
dc.contributor.author | Freeman, Daniel | - |
dc.contributor.author | Slater, Mel | - |
dc.contributor.author | Swapp, David | - |
dc.contributor.author | Antley, Angus | - |
dc.contributor.author | Barker, Chris | - |
dc.date.accessioned | 2017-08-31T09:00:57Z | - |
dc.date.available | 2017-08-31T09:00:57Z | - |
dc.date.issued | 2015 | - |
dc.identifier.issn | 1352-4658 | - |
dc.identifier.uri | https://hdl.handle.net/2445/114816 | - |
dc.description.abstract | Environmental factors have been associated with psychosis but there is little qualitative research looking at how the ongoing interaction between individual and environment maintains psychotic symptoms. Aims: The current study investigates how people with persecutory delusions interpret events in a virtual neutral social environment using qualitative methodology. Method: 20 participants with persecutory delusions and 20 controls entered a virtual underground train containing neutral characters. Under these circumstances, people with persecutory delusions reported similar levels of paranoia as non-clinical participants. The transcripts of a post-virtual reality interview of the first 10 participants in each group were analysed. Results: Thematic analyses of interviews focusing on the decision making process associated with attributing intentions of computer-generated characters revealed 11 themes grouped in 3 main categories (evidence in favour of paranoid appraisals, evidence against paranoid appraisals, other behaviour). Conclusions: People with current persecutory delusions are able to use a range of similar strategies to healthy volunteers when making judgements about potential threat in a neutral environment that does not elicit anxiety, but they are less likely than controls to engage in active hypothesis-testing and instead favour experiencing 'affect' as evidence of persecutory intention | - |
dc.format.extent | 19 p. | - |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | - |
dc.language.iso | eng | - |
dc.publisher | Cambridge University Press | - |
dc.relation.isformatof | Reproducció del document publicat a: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465813000830 | - |
dc.relation.ispartof | Behavioural And Cognitive Psychotherapy, 2015, vol. 43, p. 1-19 | - |
dc.relation.uri | https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465813000830 | - |
dc.rights | (c) British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies, 2015 | - |
dc.source | Articles publicats en revistes (Psicologia Clínica i Psicobiologia) | - |
dc.subject.classification | Paranoia | - |
dc.subject.classification | Teràpia cognitiva | - |
dc.subject.classification | Realitat virtual | - |
dc.subject.other | Paranoia | - |
dc.subject.other | Cognitive therapy | - |
dc.subject.other | Virtual reality | - |
dc.title | How do people with persecutory delusions evaluate threat in a controlled social environment? A qualitative study using virtual reality | - |
dc.type | info:eu-repo/semantics/article | - |
dc.type | info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion | - |
dc.identifier.idgrec | 656389 | - |
dc.date.updated | 2017-08-31T09:00:57Z | - |
dc.rights.accessRights | info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess | - |
dc.identifier.pmid | 24103196 | - |
Appears in Collections: | Articles publicats en revistes (Psicologia Clínica i Psicobiologia) |
Files in This Item:
File | Description | Size | Format | |
---|---|---|---|---|
656389.pdf | 361.46 kB | Adobe PDF | View/Open |
Items in DSpace are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.