Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: http://hdl.handle.net/2445/44177
Full metadata record
DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorLópez-Moliner, Joan-
dc.contributor.authorKeil, Matthias S.-
dc.date.accessioned2013-06-11T16:56:41Z-
dc.date.available2013-06-11T16:56:41Z-
dc.date.issued2012-04-
dc.identifier.issn1932-6203-
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/2445/44177-
dc.description.abstractThe visual angle that is projected by an object (e.g. a ball) on the retina depends on the object's size and distance. Without further information, however, the visual angle is ambiguous with respect to size and distance, because equal visual angles can be obtained from a big ball at a longer distance and a smaller one at a correspondingly shorter distance. Failure to recover the true 3D structure of the object (e.g. a ball's physical size) causing the ambiguous retinal image can lead to a timing error when catching the ball. Two opposing views are currently prevailing on how people resolve this ambiguity when estimating time to contact. One explanation challenges any inference about what causes the retinal image (i.e. the necessity to recover this 3D structure), and instead favors a direct analysis of optic flow. In contrast, the second view suggests that action timing could be rather based on obtaining an estimate of the 3D structure of the scene. With the latter, systematic errors will be predicted if our inference of the 3D structure fails to reveal the underlying cause of the retinal image. Here we show that hand closure in catching virtual balls is triggered by visual angle, using an assumption of a constant ball size. As a consequence of this assumption, hand closure starts when the ball is at similar distance across trials. From that distance on, the remaining arrival time, therefore, depends on ball's speed. In order to time the catch successfully, closing time was coupled with ball's speed during the motor phase. This strategy led to an increased precision in catching but at the cost of committing systematic errors.-
dc.format.extent8 p.-
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdf-
dc.language.isoeng-
dc.publisherPublic Library of Science (PLoS)-
dc.relation.isformatofReproducció del document publicat a: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0035705-
dc.relation.ispartofPLoS One, 2012, vol. 7, num. 4, p. 1-8-
dc.relation.urihttp://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0035705-
dc.rightscc-by (c) López i Moliner, Joan et al., 2012-
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/es-
dc.sourceArticles publicats en revistes (Cognició, Desenvolupament i Psicologia de l'Educació)-
dc.subject.classificationPercepció visual-
dc.subject.classificationPsicomotricitat-
dc.subject.classificationTemps de reacció (Psicologia)-
dc.subject.otherVisual perception-
dc.subject.otherPsychology of movement-
dc.subject.otherReaction time (Psychology)-
dc.titlePeople favour imperfect catching by assuming a stable world-
dc.typeinfo:eu-repo/semantics/article-
dc.typeinfo:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion-
dc.identifier.idgrec611413-
dc.date.updated2013-06-11T16:56:42Z-
dc.rights.accessRightsinfo:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess-
dc.identifier.pmid22558205-
Appears in Collections:Articles publicats en revistes (Cognició, Desenvolupament i Psicologia de l'Educació)

Files in This Item:
File Description SizeFormat 
611413.pdf304.15 kBAdobe PDFView/Open


This item is licensed under a Creative Commons License Creative Commons